logo

66 pages 2 hours read

Robert M. Sapolsky

Why Zebras Don't Get Ulcers: A Guide to Stress, Stress Related Diseases, and Coping

Nonfiction | Reference/Text Book | Adult | Published in 1993

A modern alternative to SparkNotes and CliffsNotes, SuperSummary offers high-quality Study Guides with detailed chapter summaries and analysis of major themes, characters, and more.

Chapters 9-12Chapter Summaries & Analyses

Chapter 9 Summary: “Stress and Pain”

Sapolsky references the novel Catch-22 by Joseph Heller, including an excerpt from the text in which two characters are talking about God and the existence of pain. Pain can prevent significant injuries or death by alerting people to dangerous circumstances, such as being too close to fire, and encouraging them to make an adjustment, or it can promote healing by encouraging an injured person to remain immobile. Some pain, however, is pointless, such as pain caused by cancer.

Pain originates as a sensory perception in pain receptors, which are located throughout the body. The information is transmitted to the spinal cord, where it can instigate a spinal reflex that results in rapid muscle movement, and it is sent to the brain. Pain perception is impacted by other factors, including simultaneous sensations. Pain sensations are transmitted along “fast fibers” and “slow fibers.” Fast fibers result in acute pain and a sudden movement to get away from the painful stimulus, while slow-fiber pain results in dull, throbbing types of pain. Throbbing pain can be temporarily blocked by acute pain, which is why massaging sore muscles or scratching an insect bite feels good.

Allodynia is a condition in which pain is perceived in response to non-painful stimuli or when pain in perceived after nerve damage. When pain signals reach the brain, the brain can alter the perception of the sensation, such as occurs when a soldier is wounded but does not notice the injury. Similar phenomena can occur in less-dramatic situations, as seen in patients who reported less pain after being hypnotized and asked to put their hand in hot water. The brain’s response to pain can also alter how the spinal cord reacts to the sensation.

Analgesia, or the inability to feel pain, can also originate in the brain in response to stress. This commonly happens during strenuous exercise. Opiate pain medications mimic analgesia, as the chemicals used in the drugs are similar to naturally-occurring opioids in the body, namely endorphins, enkephalins, and dynorphins. Acupuncture and the placebo effect work similarly, as both stimulate the release of opioid compounds.

Stress can also result in hyperalgesia, or the heightened psychological sensitivity to pain: “It’s the more emotional parts of the brain that are hyperreactive, the parts of the brain that are the core of our anxieties and fears” (198). Hyperalgesia can be reduced with anti-anxiety medications. Sapolsky notes the conflicting information that pain can either increase or decrease one’s pain sensitivity. He posits that stress-induced analgesia occurs with significant trauma, while hyperalgesia results from extended stress, such as feeling pain in a newly- discovered lump when you cannot yet get to the doctor and have it examined.

Sapolsky introduces fibromyalgia, which is a condition in which people experience pain seemingly without a cause. Although it has traditionally been viewed as a psychological condition, newer research suggests an underlying biological cause may be increased levels of glucocorticoids resulting in stress-induced hyperalgesia. Sapolsky writes that there are no known diseases related to analgesia or hyperalgesia.

Chapter 10 Summary: “Stress and Memory”

Stress can either enhance or obstruct memory, with acute, mild-to-moderate stressors often boosting memory, while long-term or severe stressors inhibiting memory.

Memory is often discussed in terms of short-term and long-term memories. Remote memories, or childhood memories, are a subtype of long-term memory. Sapolsky also distinguishes explicit or declarative memory, which deals with factual information, and implicit or procedural memory, which deals with remembering how to do things, like riding a bike. Memories can swap between explicit and implicit, such as occurs when a musician learns a new piece of music—it is challenging at first, but with repetition, the process of playing becomes automatic. Implicit memories can also transition to being processed explicitly, which occurs when one is encouraged to pay attention to the details of how they perform an automatic task: “Imagine descending a flight of stairs in an explicit manner” (205).

Two of the primary memory regions of the brain are the cortex and the hippocampus. Sapolsky compares the hippocampus to a keyboard and the cortex to a hard drive—the hippocampus helps input or retrieve memories from the cortex memory bank. The critical role of the hippocampus in memory was fully realized in the 1950s when a patient with epilepsy, H. M., had a significant portion of his hippocampus removed and lost the ability to form long-term explicit memories.

Scientists used to think memories were stored in individual neurons, but they now propose that memories are stored in networks, or within patterns of neural activity. Memories can be strengthened through repeated activation. The repeated activation results in more neurotransmitters, like glutamate, being released, which enhances neuronal excitation. Eventually, the glutamate excitation reaches a high-enough level for learning or memorization to take place.

Short, moderate stressors enhance emotional memories, but the accuracy of the memories is not reliable. During stress, the sympathetic nervous system delivers extra energy to the brain, which enhances the ability to retrieve and to store explicit memories. Glucocorticoids help transfer memories from short-term to long-term memories, and stress boosts sensory perception. Due to activation of the amygdala, a region of the brain dealing with fear and anxiety, emotional memory is also boosted during stress.

With severe or prolonged stress, explicit memory is inhibited, particularly the memory retrieval process. Glucocorticoids interfere with explicit memory, as can be seen in Cushing’s syndrome. Individuals with Cushing’s syndrome have high levels of glucocorticoids secreted in response to tumors, and they can develop Cushingoid dementia, which impacts explicit memory. Medically-administered glucocorticoids can cause memory issues over time. Prolonged stress also disrupts the function of the hippocampus, as neural networks within the hippocampus can atrophy; the formation of new neurons, or neurogenesis, is obstructed; and some neurons may die off.

Studies, including one conducted by Sapolsky, have shown that high levels of glucocorticoid cause the hippocampus to degenerate at a faster rate than normally seen with aging, while low glucocorticoid levels appear to delay degeneration with age. Hippocampal atrophy has been documented in people with Cushing’s syndrome, post-traumatic stress disorder, major depression, and multiple bouts of jet lag, and it has been documented as a typical occurrence in aging. High levels of glucocorticoids also correlate with more neurological impairments for stroke patients.

Sapolsky notes that more research is necessary to deepen scientists’ understandings of these correlations. Some studies also demonstrate that hippocampal atrophy can reverse if glucocorticoid levels are reduced. Sapolsky suggests more research be conducted to determine the cognitive implications of natural and medical glucocorticoids, which are prescribed for an array of conditions, including autoimmune diseases and allergies. The consequences of the stress-response during prolonged stress are likely due to the human body not yet having adapted to modern chronic stress or neurological injuries.

Chapter 11 Summary: “Stress and a Good Night’s Sleep”

Sapolsky reflects on a particularly difficult and sleepless night he and his wife spent attempting to soothe their newborn son. Sleep is a stress catch-22: Lack of sleep is a stressor, and stress disrupts sleep. Sleep occurs in different stages: shallow sleep, comprised of Stages 1 and 2 sleep; deep sleep, comprised of Stages 3 and 4; and rapid-eye movement, or REM sleep. Stage 4 sleep is also referred to as “slow wave sleep.” Sleep stages generally occur in repeated 90-minute cycles. Many parts of the brain slow down or are isolated during slow wave sleep, and brain activity increases during REM sleep.

However, activity in the frontal cortex, where reasoning and emotional filtering take place, reduces, which is why dreams are often erratic and illogical. The brain uses large amounts of energy, and sleep is critical as it allows the brain to flush out waste, to rebuild energy supplies, and to dream. Dreams, studies suggest, help to exercise important but underused neuronal pathways. Sleep benefits problem-solving skills and memory consolidation, although sleep deprivation boosts performance on reversal tasks—those that ask someone to do something in a reverse order, like reciting the alphabet backwards.

Lack of quality sleep is a stressor. Although the sympathetic nervous system is active during REM sleep, the parasympathetic nervous system is active during slow wave sleep, which is the predominant and most restful stage of sleep. Sleep deprivation results in more sympathetic nervous system activity and increased levels of glucocorticoids. The increased glucocorticoids are associated with declines in cognitive performance.

Modern society requires sleep deprivation from a segment of the population: “We’re accustomed to all sorts of amenities in our modern lives: overnight deliveries of packages, advice nurses who can be called at two in the morning, round-the-clock technical support staff” (234). Day workers also experience disruptions to their sleep cycle: The average person sleeps 1.5 hours less than the average person in 1910, due, in part, to 24-hour access to sources of entertainment and the intense pressure to be as financially productive as possible.

Stress often results in less or lower-quality sleep. The CRH released in response to stress disrupts sleep by exciting the brain’s anxiety and arousal pathways, and glucocorticoids can obstruct memory consolidation during sleep. However, biological sleep requirements generally overpower stress-based sleep difficulties, and sleep deprivation is a relatively minor stressor.

Stress hormones contribute to the waking process. One study showed that stress hormones rise about an hour before a person wakes, whether they sleep as long as they want or are woken up at a specified time. The unhealthiest sleeping condition is when sleep is unpredictably fragmented, such as nighttime emergency service providers experience.

Chapter 12 Summary: “Aging and Death”

Sapolsky posits that “the great secret of our species” is that “we will die and we know it” (239). The dread of aging and death does not exist in every culture. Sapolsky discusses an elderly man he met from the Masai tribe in East Africa. The man was scarred and had poor vision and a leg infection that Sapolsky treated. The man was accompanied by a woman who talked about the man’s youth and impending death without grief or anxiety. Although many in developed countries fear death, the aging process is usually smooth, with less disability and institutionalizations than one might expect. Some cognitive skills and the quality of relationships improve with aging, and levels of happiness increase.

As a person ages, they become less tolerant of stress. Sometimes their stress-response is diminished. This has both physical and cognitive implications. For instance, older people have more difficulty regulating their body temperatures under stressful conditions. They perform equal to younger individuals on intelligence tests with no time limit but poorer if a time limit is imposed. It is often more difficult for older bodies to stop the stress-response. Stress hormones are secreted after the immediate stressor has passed, and stress hormone levels, in general, are often higher than in younger individuals. This results in implications such as obstructed hippocampal neurogenesis.

Stress can also impact the aging process, and, in some species, excessive glucocorticoids cause death in elderly subjects. Salmons die as a result from the high levels of glucocorticoids secreted during the spawning process. That glucocorticoids cause the death of salmon is proven through experiments in which their adrenal glands were removed after spawning, and the fish survived for a year. However, this is not the case in humans and many other species, although the body’s ability to regulate glucocorticoid levels diminishes. This is likely due to dysfunctional hippocampal neurons that are less sensitive to glucocorticoids and, thus, are less effective at sending signals to stop glucocorticoid secretion.

Hippocampal damage is often caused by glucocorticoids, and hippocampal damage results in increased levels of glucocorticoids. Not every individual experiences these traits of aging, but Sapolsky notes that successful human aging will be discussed in Chapter 18. He also argues that scientists are likely years away from definitively knowing the full extent of how glucocorticoids impact the aging brain. Sapolsky suggests that, at this point, the reader may be “frazzled” by the imposing information presented so far, and he notes the rest of the book will shift its focus to stress management.

Chapters 9-12 Analysis

Chapters 9 through 12 continue to focus on explaining The Biology and Effects of Stress and the subsequent implications of modern psychological stress. Sapolsky provides readers with foundational knowledge that will help them to better understand how negative implications arise, laying the groundwork for the theme of Strategies for Stress Management and Prevention. This foundational knowledge is helpful for portraying the complexity of the stress-response, which at times seems contradictory: For instance, stress can either intensify or reduce one’s perception of pain.

Since stress can sometimes have various results, Sapolsky addresses the gaps in science that result in continued uncertainty. He also uses speculative language when he is filling in such gaps with hypotheses. Such speculative language is seen in his discussion on the rise in stress hormones that occurs before waking, and he speculates that, if people were told they would be woken at some point but not told when, their stress hormones would be relatively elevated throughout the night: “It’s quite possible that stress hormone levels will be elevated throughout the night, in nervous anticipation of that wake-up call” (238). The language choices of “quite possible” and “will be” demonstrate that no such studies have been performed and that his theory is hypothetical.

The Implications of Modern Chronic Stress are expressed through catch-22s. The catch-22s, or inescapable dilemmas, are introduced through the reference to Joseph Heller’s novel, Catch-22 in Chapter 9. Pain is a catch-22 situation because pain is unpleasant but also sometimes necessary, because it alerts people to harmful situations. Another dilemma occurs in the following chapter on memory, where stress activates the amygdala, making it more likely that stressful memories will be implicitly stored. This implicit storage of fearful memories makes the amygdala more sensitive, which, in turn, can increase stress sensitivity.

The relationship between sleep and stress can also result in a catch-22: “Not getting enough sleep is a stressor; being stressed makes it harder to sleep. Yup, we’ve got a dread vicious cycle on our hands” (227). Similarly, aging can cause stress, and stress can speed aging. These discussions introduce the relationship between The Implications of Modern Chronic Stress and Strategies for Stress Management and Prevention by illustrating that part of stress management includes mitigating these dilemmas.

Sapolsky criticizes modern society for creating sources of intense psychological stress. He writes:

When there’s the lure of 24-hour-a-day fun, activities, and entertainment or, for the workaholic, the knowledge that somewhere, in some time zone, someone else is working while you indulge yourself in sleep, that pull of ‘just a few more minutes,’ of pushing yourself, becomes irresistible. And damaging (235).

Sapolsky suggests that humans are causing their increased rates of stress and subsequent stress-related diseases through the perpetuation of a frenetic modern lifestyle. However, this message is also hopeful: If humans can cause their own sources of stress, then they can reduce or eliminate those sources of stress by altering problematic social structures.

Sapolsky applies the same implication to the stress of aging, remarking, “It’s as if we were trapped in a mine, shouting for rescuers, Save us, we’re alive but we’re getting old and we’re going to die” (239). This suggests that the inevitability of death is artificially worsened through an anxious fear of it, which Sapolsky regards as more of a cultural phenomenon than an inherent trait. He shares an anecdote about the views of death and aging in an African Masai tribe, where aging is respected and death is not feared. Again, the message becomes that this source of anxiety can be reduced through a shift in perspective, which further develops Strategies for Stress Management and Prevention.

blurred text
blurred text
blurred text
blurred text