logo

60 pages 2 hours read

Mahzarin Banaji, Anthony Greenwald

Blindspot: Hidden Biases of Good People

Nonfiction | Book | Adult | Published in 2013

A modern alternative to SparkNotes and CliffsNotes, SuperSummary offers high-quality Study Guides with detailed chapter summaries and analysis of major themes, characters, and more.

Themes

Hidden Biases

Hidden biases is not only one of the book’s key terms, it’s also one of the main themes. The book centers on the idea that most people hold prejudices of which they are unaware. While a “small minority” (209) of individuals are “overtly prejudice” (209), according to the authors, many others hold implicit biases. The authors demonstrate that these biases can be revealed through a psychology test known as the Implicit Association Test (IAT), which was developed in the 1990s by Anthony G. Greenwald—who is also one of the authors. Greenwald went on to develop the Race IAT, which measures racial bias, but other subsequent IATs have been devised to examine biases for gender, age, and other social categories.

The authors understand hidden biases as a type of “blindspot” or “mindbug”—an error in judgment based on one’s “ingrained habits of thought” (4). They place hidden biases at the heart of most discriminatory behavior in society—arguing that discrimination in general is primarily inadvertent. In other words, people do not intend to be prejudiced. They do not engage in “overtly racial hostile actions” (52) but are instead plagued by implicit negative judgments that directly affect their behavior. The authors consider the act of stereotyping as a type of hidden bias. Through years of conditioning—often from movies, books, and other types of media—people automatically associate certain groups of people with negative attributes. These “hidden stereotypes” (107) can have potentially fatal costs—as when police officers shoot an unarmed Black American. By associating “Black” with “weapons” (107), they may mistake a cell phone or other nondeadly object for a gun. However, according to the authors, many people “do not intend to impose stereotype-based costs to anybody” (102). They bear “no malign intent” (107).

While the authors discuss a number of hidden biases based on social categories, such as gender, age, and class, they focus primarily on racial biases. They argue that hidden racial prejudices contribute significantly to the clear disadvantages experienced by minority groups—particularly Black Americans. The authors argue that hidden biases may actually play a greater role in perpetuating inequality than overt ones, which are expressed by “an ever-decreasing minority of Americans” (187). Motivated by hidden stereotypes, individuals often privilege members of their own group at the expense of everyone else. Without necessarily realizing it, they are “selectively ready or able to help only or mostly those who are like them” (160). Their exclusivity works to create, maintain, and exacerbate social divisions, particularly those predicated on race. Collectively, their behavior potentially does more damage than the actions of a small minority of explicit racists.

By pointing out hidden biases, the authors hope to call greater attention to them. They view implicit bias as a grave social ill with dire consequences—driving division, tension, and conflict. Hidden prejudices can also act in self-defeating ways, as members of a stereotyped group may internalize those stereotypes. A person may decide not to pursue a certain career path, or fail to live up to their potential, by assuming the validity of a stereotype about their group. The authors discuss potential strategies for temporarily overcoming hidden biases but “(presently) lack optimism about fully eradicating mindbugs” (167).

The Power of the Unconscious Mind

A major theme running through the book is that the unconscious mind has tremendous power to guide our thoughts, actions, and behaviors. According to the authors, hidden biases exist without our awareness and can only be exposed to us through methods like the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Despite our inability to recognize them in our daily lives, they are incredibly powerful. They may lead us to stereotype and exclude others, thereby perpetuating systems of discrimination already in place. That unconscious thoughts have so much influence is a prevailing idea in contemporary psychology—having replaced previous beliefs that “human behavior was primarily guided by conscious thoughts and feelings” (xiv). The notion of hidden biases directly reinforces and perpetuates current ideology. By conveying a set of influential biases as hidden, the authors link them directly to the unconscious.

The authors explain that there are two sides of the mind: the reflective and the automatic. The reflective side is the conscious side, and it plays a critical role in our daily decision-making. We consciously form judgments and preferences that influence our actions. For example, we choose a political affiliation and vote for a particular candidate. However, we also have an automatic side that likewise forms judgments and preferences that influence our actions. The automatic side is the unconscious side, and it’s where hidden biases are housed. While the power dynamic between the two sides is not entirely clear, the automatic side certainly holds its own. The authors explain that the automatic side can undermine conscious intentions and lead us to act in unexpected ways or “toward less conscious decisions” (55). We may set out or intend to do one thing and end up doing something entirely different—without ever recognizing why.

The power of the automatic or unconscious mind is perhaps the most pervasive idea in the book. Hidden biases matter because automatic thoughts can compete with, or override, reflective thoughts. People can say they believe in equality for everyone but still practice subtle forms of discrimination without recognizing it. Other concepts in the book also depend on the power of the unconscious. Stereotyping is often a product of the automatic mental associations we draw between a group of people and an idea. Because unconscious thoughts have so much influence over us, the authors remain skeptical that people can completely overcome their hidden biases. They worry about the “many routine, daily social interactions” (167) in which implicit biases put others at a disadvantage.

However, the authors believe that recognizing one’s hidden biases—and the power of the unconscious—is an important first step, and they provide several IATs in the book, encouraging the reader to take them.

Discriminatory Behavior

The authors continuously push the connection between hidden biases and discriminatory behavior. They do so because they view discrimination motivated by implicit prejudices as potentially more consequential in creating and perpetuating Black disadvantage than overt racism. According to the authors, the number of people who are openly racist comprises only a “small proportion of people” (209). Conversely, a vast majority of the people who take the Race IAT score a white preference—which predicts discrimination. The authors therefore find it “reasonable to conclude” (209) that implicit bias is perhaps the most significant contributor to Black disadvantage.

After author Anthony (Tony) G. Greenwald devised the Race IAT in the mid-1990s, a slew of research studies connecting IAT results to discriminatory behavior followed. They found that scoring a white preference on the Race IAT served as a “moderate predictor of racially discriminatory behavior” (52). The scenarios simulated in research experiments included job interviews, medical treatments and recommendations, and assessing Black and white faces. A white preference on the Race IAT skewed research participants toward favoring white job applicants, white medical patients, and white faces.

The authors primarily focus on racial discrimination toward Black Americans. The appendices include discussions of how racial attitudes toward Black Americans have changed as well as an examination of the types of contemporary disadvantages Black Americans experience. The authors conclude, based on research, that disadvantages exist in housing, health care, hiring, and criminal justice, and are partially a result of discrimination rather than other causes. While racism is not as overt as it once was, the authors argue that it is still very much present in contemporary society. It simply exists more covertly through hidden biases that influence behavior.

In many ways, the book attempts to serve as a wake-up call to those who, like the authors, embrace equality but score a white preference on the Race IAT. The authors clearly indicate that such a score most likely translates into discriminatory behavior on the part of the test taker. Privileging those who are similar by practicing “in-group favoritism” (162), or failing to help those who are different serve as ways one might be inadvertently perpetuating disadvantage. While discrimination can be directed toward all types of people, the authors make clear that discriminatory behavior based on race—built on hidden biases—is particularly pervasive.

Self-Awareness

Throughout the book, the authors encourage the reader to become more self-aware of his or her biases. They explain in the preface that their “highest aim” (xv) is to substantiate hidden biases and their consequences with scientific evidence so that the “good people” (xv) of the book’s title will be able to “align their behavior with their intentions” (xv). When behavior is guided by prejudices of which we are unaware it may guide us to act in ways we do not intend. Awareness of one’s hidden biases is arguably a path to gaining more self-control.

There are a few ways the authors encourage self-awareness. They provide several Implicit Association Tests (IATs) for the reader to take throughout the book. These tests measure implicit biases by revealing mental associations between people and ideas or objects. Central to the book’s IAT samples is the Race IAT, which indicates if one has a preference for one race over another. While scoring an “automatic White preference” (46) on the Race IAT does not necessarily mean the test-taker is prejudiced, it is a “moderate predictor” (52) of discriminatory behavior. The authors support this connection—between the Race IAT and discriminatory behavior—with several research studies. Although people may not be happy to learn they have a hidden race bias, the IAT makes “it possible to look into their blindspot” (52)—revealing “a new awareness of a conflict between conscious and unconscious thoughts or feelings ” (59). Seeing our prejudices takes us a step closer to overcoming them.

The authors also demonstrate how we might be acting in ways counter to our intentions. In Chapter 2, “Shades of Truth,” they ask us to consider the idea that we constantly lie without being aware of it. They show us how we can “underestimate the truth substantially” (24), or even lie to ourselves in various circumstances. We may also believe in the essential truth of a statement that is actually false. Gaining a sense of our “unrecognized untruths” (20) as well as their causes shows how we may potentially distort our sense of our racial beliefs and attitudes—both to others and to ourselves.

By providing IATs and asking direct questions that force us to confront our untruths, the authors reinforce their goal of helping the reader recognize and understand their own hidden biases. They want us to see that no one is immune from holding stereotypes and prejudices or engaging in discriminatory behavior despite the fact that our conscious beliefs may be to the contrary. Cognitive dissonance, or a conflict between our beliefs and actions, produces discomfort and “violates” (59) our ability to achieve harmony. Becoming self-aware about our hidden biases can help us overcome our cognitive dissonance and reach mental consonance.

blurred text
blurred text
blurred text
blurred text